CRITICISM - LEARNING TO LOOK |
Learning
to look 24
Hours, August 1994, pp. 52-55 and
124. On sale at the entrance
desk of the National Gallery of Australia is a booklet entitled Sixty Minutes in the Gallery. Suppose you looked at only the 48
works illustrated in this guide and ignored the dozen more mentioned in
the text. That decision would allow 75 seconds to find and then examine
each object. The length of time spent with Jackson Pollock’s “Blue
Poles” or the 200 poles of the Aboriginal memorial, therefore, would
be half a minute. Similarly, in Madrid, a brochure for the
Thyssen-Bornemisza Collection suggests that entrants allow at least 90
minutes to see 500 items – nine seconds for each painting or
sculpture. A recent world-wide
survey of art museum audiences found that nine seconds was the average
time which visitors spent before each piece, down from 30 seconds a few
years ago. What can be absorbed in nine seconds of glancing? Enough,
perhaps, if you have not earned how to look. Once those skills are
acquired, however, nine minutes will seem too short for work such as
Poussin’s “The Crossing of the Red Sea” in the National Gallery of
Victoria. Since almost all of us
can see, why do we need to learn how to look? None of us expects to read
poetry in a language we have never learned. Nor do we suppose we can
follow a musical score without first studying its system of notation.
Such modesty is not automatic among gallery-goers. One reason for this
reluctance is that we can talk about most images without needing to
acquire a new vocabulary or technical competence. Yet, without those
skills, judgements about paintings are, as Margaret Preston put it,
limited to approving of a picture because a tree in it looks like the
one under which grandma died. One barrier to
understanding the visual arts is the supposition that what we see is the
result of an untutored ability possessed by everyone who does not have
defective vision. This assumption that sight is natural makes
gallery-goers suspect they are being put down when they do not apprehend
what curators claim to see in a painting. But the difference is one of
learning, not of innate sensibility. Powers of concentration and
observation are enhanced by training. Kipling has the boy hero of Kim practice recall to speed up his identification of objects.
Gallery-going calls for comparable preparation. Being taught how to see
is not the same as being told what to enjoy. Experts retain their
preferences and so should we all. Scholars who rave about Titian may
shake their heads at the mention of Tintoretto, just as lovers of
chamber music can abominate opera. However, learning to
look will increase the range of pictures we can enjoy. For example, some
viewers still see landscape paintings by Fred Williams (1927-1982) as
instances of abstract minimalism. Those lucky enough to have attended
the 1988 retrospective for Williams did not need anyone to explain that
that judgement is far from the whole truth. Walking through an
exhibition of 300 canvases produced across 30 years, viewers traveled
with Williams as he refined his vision of Australian scenery. His 1950s
paintings were obviously of trees. Within a decade he had focussed on a
few key elements from the countryside on the outskirts of Melbourne,
such as dirt roads, stumps, logs, fences and electricity poles. His art
was never abstract, but often abstracted. To encounter a late Williams
by itself can be puzzling because it has a few bolts of colour riveted
to a horizonless background. Once those marks are recognised as the
residue of European occupation, a Williams painting is as
representational as a Streeton. Indeed, many are literal in their
portrayals of how we have despoiled the bush. What to do after
passing through the gallery entrance way? Tour parties have two opposed
strategies for exploring museums. The popular option is
to patter through all the galleries without pausing, except to take
photographs. Gallery-goes seem to use cameras as defensive weapons.
Taking snaps becomes the preferred way of seeing when one does not know
how to perceive. The camera does the looking and the long life of the
photograph replaces the need to ponder the original, with the machine
supposedly achieving osmosis between the object and the brain without
the effort of perception. If tourists want high-quality reminders, they
would be better served buying slides or postcards from the museum shop. The more rewarding
approach, however, is to march past most of the pictures on the way to a
nine-minute lecture in front of a key image such as Titian’s
“Assumption” in the Frari in Venice. The lecture can come from a
paid guide, a hired audio cassette or a printed brochure. No matter how much time
one has, some degree of selection will always be necessary. At one
extreme, one can follow the arrows in the Louvre to the “Mona Lisa”
and then follow the arrows to the Exit. A different approach would be to
start with a 90-minute stroll the North-European wing before taking tea
and deciding exactly where to pass the hours available. (In museums as
huge as the Louvre, time will permit such a preliminary survey of the
paintings in only one wing.) Next comes a 90-minute session of slow
looking in the chosen rooms. Never underestimate how tiring it can be to
look at art. Half-hour tea breaks every couple of hours are essential to
maintain concentration. One sidelight in the refurbished Louvre is that
cafes (and lavatories) have been located on most floors in each wing, so
that treks back to main foyer are not necessary, as at the Prado. Stage two is to decide
which paintings warrant more time. None of us is so blasé about hype
that we do not want to see the most famous works in each collection –
whether Picasso’s “Guernica” (1937) or the Barberini “Faun”
(2250BP) in Munich. After those devotions, it helps to pursue a few
themes so as not to become lost among the thousands of images. For
instance, you might keep note of how painters depicted the sun and snow;
or attend to certain legends such as Judith’s decapitation of
Holofernes. Concentration on two or three topics will help to break
through the surface of each work. Each line of inquiry will, during the
years, spill on to others, such as the discovery of a favourite
sculptor, Giambologna perhaps. Stage three concerns
how to look into the pictures selected. Some device is necessary to
penetrate beyond the subject matter. Before anyone told me better, I
used to investigate pictures by latching onto a prominent feature and
working outwards from it before leaping across to another highlight. The
result was to encounter the canvas as a patchwork, a hit-or-miss process
which allowed the study of some aspects in detail, while others remained
blurred. Just how difficult it
is to see all the imagery in a painting came back to me recently when I
was giving a lunchtime talk about Tom Roberts’s “Bourke Street”
(1885-91), a painting I thought I knew thoroughly. While lecturing in
front of the picture, I recognised for the first time that the cart in
the middle of the intersection had the word ICE painted on it. Was this
Roberts’s joke against the dry heat of the scene? After the audience
had left I returned with paper and pen to make a new list of its
contents. It was actually David
Jaffe who taught me this annotation process. (David was later appointed
curator of European paintings at the Getty in Los Angeles and then at
the National Gallery in London.) David’s tuition began in 1979 when we
were both teaching Fine Art at the University of Queensland and
continued after he moved to Canberra to work at the National Gallery.
When we met, he was intrigued by Raphael’s portrait of Pope Julius II
(c 1511), and I remember hi showing me how to look at that image. Of
course, because we were in Brisbane, we had to work from a colour slide
to make my mind go beyond he picture’s obvious subject, which meant
breaking his first rule of art appreciation: to look at the picture
itself. When a student at the Courtauld, he had been perplexed that many
scholars spent their time in the library poring over black-and-white
photographs of canvases which were on display a few hundred metres down
Charing Cross Road. With “Julius II”,
the task was to discern the portrait’s pictorial and iconographical
content, and David suggested that I would identify more of the elements
Raphael had incorporated if I move my line of vision across the surface
from left to right, before going from top to bottom; I should then
repeat the process in the opposite directions. Some canvases are too big
to scan in one go and need to be divided into four or six segments,
before moving our eyes up and down and from side to side across those
areas. Such close attention to
the Raphael raised questions – what, for example, was the significance
of the Pope’s beard, handkerchief and ring? Answers lay outside the
picture’s frame, since those accessories were elements of the
propaganda that His Holiness used to make himself appear as a second
Julius Caesar. So, a further stage of
art appreciation requires library research. How important this can be is
exemplified by the scholarship that David applied to the Rubens
self-portrait he secured for the National Gallery in Canberra. Working
through Latin manuscripts in the Bibliotheque Nationale and at the Vatican, he proved that the
version he had bought was the commissioned original, while the one at
Windsor Castle was Rubens’s own copy. When the Getty Museum employed
David Jaffe for his “eye”, they were acquiring his well-stocked mind
and visual memory. Few gallery-goers need
to pursue research at such a level. Yet no one can see Western art
without some basic knowledge of Christianity. To tour the major European
galleries is to encounter hectares of paintings inspired by
Christianity, mostly out of Roman Catholic theologies. Protestants were
less tempted to idolatry. Time was when it was the exceptional child who
knew of Christmas only as the day before the races out at Tanmalangmaloo.
Nowadays, the Bible is a closed book to many people. A young woman
asking bout a Rembrandt portrait of Christ exclaimed “How weird!” on
having the crown of thorns explained to her. Appreciation of art
history is important too. For example, David was still in Brisbane when
I returned a few weeks in 1981. By then, I was researching Tom Roberts
for a television drama series which the ABC produced in 1984 as One
Summer Again. When David looked over my shoulder at a reproduction
of Roberts’s Shearing the Rams, he pointed to the youth carrying away
the fleece and remarked, “He’s from Ghiberti’s ‘Gates of
Paradise’.” (1420s) After I replied that the experts claimed his
stance derived from the boy in Courbet’s “The Stonebreaker”
(1849), David demonstrated why a knowledge of Renaissance art is
invaluable for historians of Australian painting. His published
researches on that flame-like figure has challenged art historians to
think again, not only about “Shearing the Rams” but also about
whether Roberts was more Classicist than Realist throughout his career. Looking leads to
note-taking. Just as it is rewarding to read with a pen in hand, so it
is helpful to look at pictures similarly equipped. A notepad computer
would be ideal – except that it cannot take in drawings and even the
roughest sketches help to divine the structure of a painting, as well as
to recall its patterning and tones. An A4 exercise book with a spiral
spine is a good option, as it allows pages to be removed easily (some
institutions will not allow visitors to take in clip-boards). Each
painting gets its own page, headed with the name of the artist, title of
the work, location and dates of composition and viewing. Then you make
inventories of its staffage (the “accessory items” in a picture –
the figures in a landscape, for example), its brushstrokes or its
chromaticism. During each rest break, these notes can be scanned to
determine to which paintings you want to return. The notes will also be
the keys to unlocking patterns; for example, while collating the pages
about Jose Ribera (1591-1652), I realised how he had used elongated arms
to create Mannerist effects. The pleasures of
looking are not limited to the interpretation of imagery. Practising
painters complain that scholars know a lot about art but not much about
painting. Yet the two cannot be separated. The way a scene is painted is
as much a part of its meaning as is any heraldic of mythological
attribute. Consider how the significance of a Rembrandt self-portrait
would be altered if he had laid down paint as thickly as van Gogh did in
his last year. The methods outlined
above for studying images can be applied to examining passages of paint,
whether for textures or colours. Notes about these aspects also include
rough sketches showing the direction of the drag marks of the brush.
This can be crucial in a Monet, whose pictures of the coast at Belle-Ile,
for example, are divided into four of five large areas of sea, cliffs
and sky. Not only are the colours of these elements different but within
each block he set the paint at varying angles. If the direction of one
set of strokes were to change, the picture would fall apart. No traveler has enough
time or the stamina to study every detail in every painting in every
museum. One solution is to track a handful of characters who turn up
repeatedly, such as St Anne or St Joseph. And the manner of depicting
Balthazar, the blackest of the Magi, can give some measure of the
intensities of colour prejudice in European cultures – is he, for
example, the only Magus kneeling? Watching out for
representations of these legends can revive your attention while moving
from room to room. More importantly, the presence in a picture of one of
your chosen figures provides a point of entry into the rest of its
iconology. Having studied the treatment of St Anne in a score of
paintings, for example, it becomes possible to use the way she is
handled on an unfamiliar canvas to make sense of the hierarchy of
relations between the accompanying figures; the colours of her garments
will offer additional clues. Tracing the depictions
of St Sebastian can also demonstrate how a variety of issues can be
gathered along a single thread. The prevalence of images of a near-naked
young man with arrows sticking out of him has been interpreted as a
homoerotic statement form Renaissance artists expressing the Classical
idea of male love. Because the man is tied up he cannot resist the
viewers’ desire, which is represented by the arrow as penis. One
surprise is that none of the paintings shows Sebastian dead; he is
rarely even unconscious. Nor do his eyes look at the viewer, but are
either turned up to heaven or cast down to the ground. Thus, St
Sebastian offers a case study in the possessive gaze – a notion that
has been advanced by feminists in criticising a voyeuristic possession
by male viewers of female nudes. If that interpretation
holds more than a gay fantasy, it is also much less than the complete
story. For a start, in Mediaeval altar pieces, St Sebastian remained
garbed. Also, those pre-Renaissance depictions of his martyrdom came at
the end of a sequence of panels illustrating his defiance of pagan
authorities. By the Renaissance, the method of martyrdom provided a
reason to paint flesh, an opportunity otherwise available only from
the crucified Christ, whose body had to be treated with
circumspection. Artists who wanted to balance their composition of the
naked Christ with a second passage of skin tones could include St
Sebastian around the foot of the cross. The nakedness of St Lawrence was
less useful because his being roasted alive meant that he had to be
horizontal, whereas Sebastian came more or less upright. Then there are the
innumerable images of the Holy Family. In order not to glaze over at the
sight of yet another rendition, museum visitors might consider testing
Leo Steinberg’s thesis about the representation of the doctrines of
the Incarnation and Resurrection. Steinberg argues that a painter’s
revealing the genitals of the infant Jesus, sometimes with the Virgin
Mary pointing at them or even touching them, was a confirmation of
Christ’s human nature – that God indeed had become man. In contrast,
the Resurrection from the dead was indicated by depicting Christ with an
erect member, perhaps covered by cloth yet nonetheless recognisable from
the folds in that garment. Since many of these theological affirmations
were covered up during the Counter-Revolution, one’s powers of
observation are sharpened by spotting the over-paint. Caution is advisable,
however, before sharing one’s thoughts about a painting. In front of
Jacques-Louis David’s “Death of Socrates” (1787) in the New York
Metropolitan, a teenage black pointed to Socrates and asked “Who dat?”
Here was my chance to spread enlightenment. “That’s a famous Greek
philosopher, called Socrates”, went my pedagogical correctness.
“:Socrates was a black man”, came the reaction of this agent
provocateur from Malcolm X. |